The Seven Essential Virtues of TV Stars. Hypersensitivity, HSP: what is it? What is the name of sensitivity

One of the most important indicators of the quality of the receiving path is the sensitivity of the receiver. It characterizes the ability of the receiver to receive weak signals. Receiver sensitivity is defined as the minimum input signal level of the device required to provide the required quality of the received information. The quality can be estimated by a given bit error rate (BER), error message reception probability (MER), or signal-to-noise ratio SNR (Signal-to-Noise Ratio) at the input of the receiver demodulator. If the receiver sensitivity is limited, then it can be estimated by the actual or marginal receiver sensitivity, noise figure, or noise temperature.

The sensitivity of a receiver with low gain, at the output of which there is practically no noise, is determined by the emf, (or nominal power) of the signal in the antenna (or its equivalent), at which a given voltage (power) of the signal at the output of the receiver is provided.

The sensitivity of the receiver is determined by its gain K US. The receiver must provide amplification of even the weakest input signals to the output level necessary for the normal operation of the device, however, interference and noise act at the receiver input, which are also amplified in the receiver and can degrade the quality of its operation. In addition, amplified internal noise appears at the output of the receiver. The less internal noise, the better the quality of the receiver, the higher the sensitivity of the receiver.

Real Sensitivity receiver is equal to emf. (or nominal power) of the signal in the antenna, at which the voltage (power) of the signal at the output of the receiver exceeds the voltage (power) of the interference by a given number of times. Ultimate sensitivity receiver is equal to emf. or the nominal power of the RAP signal in the antenna, at which at the output of its linear part (i.e., at the input of the detector), the signal power is equal to the power of the internal noise.

When setting the sensitivity of the receiver in the form of emf, it is measured in microvolts. Modern mobile communication receivers have a sensitivity of tenths of a microvolt. The method of setting the sensitivity of the receiver in the form of emf. leads to the fact that with a different input resistance of the receiver, we will get a different emf value. Therefore, despite the fact that all modern receivers of mobile communication systems have an input impedance of 50 ohms, the sensitivity of the receivers is specified in terms of the signal power at the receiver input. Sensitivity is defined as the ratio of the power at the receiver input to the 1 mW power level and is expressed on a logarithmic scale in dBm.

The ultimate sensitivity of the receiver can also be characterized by the noise figure N 0 , equal to the ratio of the noise power generated at the output of the linear part of the receiver by the antenna equivalent (at room temperature T 0 = 290 K) and the linear part, to the noise power generated only by the antenna equivalent. Obviously,

, (1)

where k= 1.38 10 –23 J/deg is the Boltzmann constant;
Pw is the noise band of the linear part of the receiver, Hz;
R AP is the signal power, W.

From (1) it can be seen that the signal power corresponding to its limiting sensitivity and related to the frequency band unit can be expressed in units kT 0:

, (2)

The maximum sensitivity of the receiver can also be characterized by the noise temperature of the receiver T pr, for which it is necessary to additionally heat the equivalent of the antenna, so that at the output of the linear part of the receiver, the power of the noise generated by it is equal to the noise power of the linear part. Obviously, where

(3)

A real antenna is affected by external noise, the rated power of which is ,
where T A is the noise temperature of the antenna. Therefore, at the output of the linear part

To obtain equality of signal and noise powers, power is required

Literature:

  1. "Design of radio receivers" ed. A.P. Sievers - M .: "Higher School" 1976 pp. 7-8
  2. "Radio receivers" ed. Zhukovsky - M .: "Sov. Radio" 1989 pp. 8 - 10
  3. Palshkov V.V. "Radio receivers" - M .: "Radio and communication" 1984 pp. 12 - 14

Together with the article "Receiver Sensitivity" they read:

Depending on the value of the received frequency, the circuit and design solutions of radio receivers can vary significantly.
https://website/WLL/DiapPrmFr.php

Adjacent channel selectivity is the ability of a receiver to receive a useful signal at a given channel frequency with a given error probability
https://website/WLL/ChastotIzbirat.php

Intermodulation, blocking, one decibel compression point, these are the main sources of reception side channels! To know and be able to deal with these phenomena is the task of any technical specialist.
https://website/WLL/NelinPrm.php

The dynamic range of the receiver, on the one hand, determines the ability of the receiver to detect a weak input signal, on the other hand, to process high-level signals without distortion.
https://website/WLL/DinDiapPrm.php

Naturally, we are interested in reducing the probability of a type II error as much as possible, that is, increasing the sensitivity of the criterion. To do this, you need to know what it depends on. In principle, this problem is similar to the one that was solved in relation to type I errors, but with one important exception.

To evaluate the sensitivity of a test, you need to specify the amount of difference that it should detect. This value is determined by the objectives of the study. In the diuretic example, the sensitivity was low - 55%. But, perhaps, the researcher simply did not consider it necessary to detect an increase in diuresis from 1200 to 1400 ml / day, that is, by only 17%?

As the data scatter increases, the probability of both types of errors increases. As we will see shortly, it is more convenient to account for the magnitude of the differences and the spread of the data together by calculating the ratio of the magnitude of the differences to the standard deviation.

The sensitivity of a diagnostic test can be increased by reducing its specificity - a similar relationship exists between the level of significance and the sensitivity of the criterion. The higher the significance level (that is, the smaller a), the lower the sensitivity.

As we have already said, the most important factor that affects the probability of both type I and type II errors is the sample size. As the sample size increases, the probability of error decreases. In practice, this is very important, since it is directly related to the design of the experiment.

Before proceeding to a detailed consideration of the factors affecting the sensitivity of the criterion, we list them again.

Significance level a. The smaller a, the lower the sensitivity.

The ratio of the size of the differences to the standard deviation. The larger this ratio, the more sensitive the criterion.

Sample size. The larger the volume, the higher the sensitivity of the criterion.

Significance level

To get a visual representation of the relationship between the sensitivity of the criterion and the level of significance, let's return to Fig. 6.3. By choosing the significance level a, we thereby set the critical value of t. We choose this value so that the proportion of values ​​that exceed it - provided that the drug has no effect - is equal to a (Fig. 6.3A). The sensitivity of the criterion is the proportion of those values ​​of the criterion that exceed the critical one, provided that the treatment has an effect (Fig. 6.3B). As can be seen from the figure, if the critical value is changed, this share will also change.

Let's take a closer look at how this happens.

On fig. 6.4A shows the distribution of Student's t-test values. Difference from fig. 6.3 is that this is now the distribution obtained for all 1027 possible pairs of samples. The top graph is the distribution of t values ​​for the case when the drug does not have a diuretic effect. Suppose we chose a significance level of 0.05, that is, we took a = 0.05. In this case, the critical value is 2.101, which means we reject the null hypothesis and accept the differences as statistically significant at t > +2.101 or t. 6.4B. It shows the same distributions of t values. The difference in the chosen significance level is a = 0.01. The critical value of t has increased to 2.878, the dotted line has shifted to the right and cuts off only 45% of the lower plot. Thus, when moving from 5% to 1% significance level, the sensitivity decreased from 55 to 45%. Accordingly, the probability of a type II error increased to 1 - 0.45 = 0.55.

So, by reducing a, we reduce the risk of rejecting the correct null hypothesis, that is, finding differences (effect) where there are none. But by doing so, we also reduce sensitivity - the probability of detecting differences that actually exist.

Size of difference

Considering the influence of the significance level, we took the magnitude of the differences constant: our drug increased the daily diuresis from 1200 to 1400 ml, that is, by 200 ml. Now let's accept


constant significance level a = 0.05 and see how the sensitivity of the test depends on the magnitude of the differences. It is clear that large differences are easier to identify than small ones. Consider the following examples. On fig. 6.5A shows the distribution of t values ​​for the case when the study drug does not have a diuretic effect. Hatched are 5% of the largest absolute values ​​of t located to the left - 2.101 or to the right +2.101. On fig. 6.5B shows the distribution of t values ​​for the case when the drug increases the daily

Increase in daily diuresis, ml

diuresis by an average of 200 ml (we have already considered this situation). Above the right critical value lies 55% of the possible values ​​of t: the sensitivity is 0.55. Next, in fig. 6.5B shows the distribution of t values ​​for the case when the drug increases diuresis by an average of 100 ml. Now only 17% of t values ​​exceed 2.101. Thus, the sensitivity of the test is only 0.17. In other words, the effect will be found in less than one out of every five comparisons between the control and experimental groups. Finally, fig. 6.5D represents a case of increased diuresis by 400 ml. 99% of the values ​​of t fell into the critical region. The sensitivity of the test is 0.99: differences will almost certainly be detected.

By repeating this thought experiment, one can determine the sensitivity of the test for all possible effect values, from zero to "infinite". Plotting the results on a graph, we get Fig. 6.6, where the sensitivity of the test is shown as a function of the magnitude of the differences. From this graph, you can determine what the sensitivity will be for a particular effect size. So far, the graph is not very convenient to use, because it is only suitable for this group size, standard deviation, and significance level. We'll build another graph shortly that's more suitable for research planning, but first we need to understand more about the role of scatter and group size.

Scatter of values

The sensitivity of the test increases with the observed differences; as the spread of values ​​increases, the sensitivity, on the contrary, decreases.

Recall that Student's t-test is defined as follows:

where X1 and X2 are averages, s is the combined score of the standard

deviations a, n1 and n2 are sample sizes. Note that x1 and

X2 are estimates of two (different) means - p and p2. For simplicity, we assume that the volumes of both samples are equal, that is, n1 = n2. Then the computed value of t is an estimate of the quantity P1-P2 P-P


Thus, t depends on the ratio of the effect size to the standard deviation.

Let's look at a few examples. The standard deviation in our study population is 200 ml (see Fig. 6.1). In this case, an increase in daily diuresis by 200 or 400 ml is equal to one or two standard deviations, respectively. These are very noticeable changes. If the standard deviation were 50 ml, then the same changes in diuresis would be even more significant, amounting to 4 and 8 standard deviations, respectively. Conversely, if the standard deviation were, for example, 500 ml, then the change in urine output in 200 ml would be 0.4 standard deviation. Finding such an effect would be difficult and hardly worth it at all.

So, the sensitivity of the test is affected not by the absolute magnitude of the effect, but by its ratio to the standard deviation. Let's denote it f (Greek "phi"); this ratio φ = 5/a is called the noncentrality parameter.

Sample size

We have learned about two factors that affect the sensitivity of a test: the significance level a and the non-centrality parameter φ. The more a and the more f, the more feeling
validity. Unfortunately, we cannot influence at all, and as for a, its increase increases the risk of rejecting the correct null hypothesis, that is, finding differences where there are none. However, there is one more factor that we can, within certain limits, change at our discretion without sacrificing the level of significance. We are talking about the sample size (number of groups). With an increase in the sample size, the sensitivity of the test increases.

There are two reasons why increasing the sample size increases the sensitivity of the test. First, increasing the sample size increases the number of degrees of freedom, which in turn reduces the critical value. Secondly, as can be seen from the formula just obtained


the value of t grows with the sample size n (this is also true for many other criteria).

Figure 6.7A reproduces the distributions from fig. 6.4A. The upper graph corresponds to the case when the drug does not have a diuretic effect, the lower one - when the drug increases daily diuresis by 200 ml. The number of each group is 10 people. Figure 6.7B shows similar distributions. The difference is that now each group included not 10, but 20 people. Since the size of each of the groups is 20, the number of degrees of freedom is V = 2(20 - 1) = 38. From Table 4.1, we find that the critical value of t at the 5% significance level is 2.024 (in the case of samples of size 10, it was 2.101). On the other hand, an increase in the sample size led to an increase in the values ​​of the criterion. As a result, not 55, but 87% of the values ​​of t exceed the critical value. So, increasing the size of the groups from 10 to 20 people led to an increase in sensitivity from 0.55 to 0.87.

Going through all possible sample sizes, you can plot the sensitivity of the test as a function of the size of the groups (Fig. 6.8). With increasing volume sensitivity



is growing. At first, it grows rapidly, then, starting from a certain sample size, the growth slows down.

Sensitivity calculation is an essential part of medical research planning. Now, having become acquainted with the most important factor that determines sensitivity, we are ready to solve this problem.

How to determine the sensitivity of a criterion?

On fig. 6.9 the sensitivity of the Student's test is presented as a function of the non-centrality parameter f = 5/s at a significance level a = 0.05. The four curves correspond to the four sample sizes.

The samples are assumed to be of equal size. What if it's not? If you refer to Fig. 6.9 when planning a study (which is very reasonable), then you need to consider the following. For a given total number of patients, it is precisely the equal number of groups that ensures maximum sensitivity. So, an equal number of groups should be planned. If, however, you decide to calculate sensitivity after the study, when, having found no statistically significant difference, you want to determine the extent to which this can be considered evidence of no effect, then you should take the size of both groups equal to the smaller of them. This calculation will give a somewhat underestimated sensitivity, but will save you from being overly optimistic.

Let's apply curves from fig. 6.9 for example with a diuretic (see fig. 6.1). We want to calculate the sensitivity of Student's t-test at a significance level of a = 0.05. The standard deviation is 200 ml. What is the probability of detecting an increase in daily diuresis by 200 ml?

The number of control and experimental groups is ten. We choose in Fig. 6.9 the corresponding curve and find that the sensitivity of the criterion is 0.55.

So far, we have been talking about the sensitivity of the Stew test.


Sample size

Halothane and morphine in open heart surgery

In ch. In Table 4, we compared the cardiac index during halothane and morphine anesthesia (see Table 4.2) and did not find statistically significant differences. (Recall that the cardiac index is the ratio of the minute volume of the heart to the surface area of ​​the body.) However, the groups were small - 9 and 16 people. The mean CI in the halothane group was 2.08 L/min/m2; in the morphine group 1.75 l/min/m2, i.e. 16% less. Even if the differences were statistically significant, such a small difference would hardly be of any practical interest.

So let's put the question this way: what was the probability of detecting a difference of 25%? The combined variance estimate is s2 = 0.89, so the standard deviation is 0.94 l/min/m2. Twenty five percent of 2.08 l/min/m2 is 0.52 l/min/m2.

Thereby,

5 _ 0.52 o ~ 0.94

Since the sizes of the groups do not match, we will choose the smallest of them - 9 - to estimate the sensitivity. 6.9 it follows that in this case the sensitivity of the criterion is 0.16.

The chances of detecting even a 25% difference were very small. Let's summarize.

The sensitivity of a test is the probability of rejecting the false hypothesis of no difference.

The sensitivity of the test is affected by the significance level: the smaller a, the lower the sensitivity.

The larger the effect size, the greater the sensitivity.

The larger the sample size, the greater the sensitivity.

Sensitivity is calculated differently for different criteria.

Sensitivity I

the ability of the organism to perceive various stimuli emanating from the external and internal environment, and to respond to them.

Ch. is based on the processes of reception, the biological significance of which lies in the perception of stimuli acting on them, their transformation into excitation processes (Excitation) , which are the source of the corresponding sensations (pain, temperature, light, auditory, etc.). Subjectively experienced appears with threshold stimulation of certain receptors (Receptors) . In those cases when the incoming receptors in the c.n.s. below the threshold of sensation, it does not cause this or that sensation, however, it can lead to certain reflex reactions of the body (vegetative-vascular, etc.).

For understanding the physiological mechanisms of Ch., the teachings of I.P. Pavlova about analyzers (Analyzers) . As a result of the activity of all parts of the analyzer, a subtle and synthesis of stimuli acting on irritations is carried out. In this case, not only the transmission of impulses from receptors to the central analyzer occurs, but also a complex process of reverse (efferent) regulation of sensitive perception (see Self-regulation of physiological functions) . The excitability of the receptor apparatus is determined both by the absolute intensity of stimulation, and by the number of simultaneously stimulated receptors or by the quality of their repeated irritations - the law of summation of receptor irritations. the excitability of the receptor depends on the influence of the central nervous system. and sympathetic innervation.

Sensory impulses from the peripheral receptor apparatus reach the cerebral cortex along specific pathways and non-specific pathways of the reticular formation (Reticular formation) Non-specific afferent impulses travel along the spinoreticular pathway, which at the level of the brainstem (Brainstem) has connections with the cells of the reticular formation . The activating and inhibitory systems of the reticular formation (see Functional systems) carry out the regulation of afferent impulses, participate in the selection of information coming from the periphery to the higher parts of the Ch. system, passing some impulses and blocking others.

There are general and special Ch. General Ch. is divided into exteroceptive, proprioceptive and interoceptive. Exteroceptive (superficial, skin) include pain, temperature (thermal and cold) and tactile Ch. () with their varieties (for example, electrocutaneous - sensations caused by various types of electric current; feeling of moisture - hygroesthesia , it is based on a combination of tactile sensation with temperature; a feeling of itching is a variant of tactile Ch., etc.).

Proprioceptive (deep) Ch. - bathiesthesia includes muscular-articular Ch. (a sense of the position of the body and its parts in space), vibration (), pressure (). To interoceptive (vegetative-visceral) is Ch., associated with the receptor apparatus in the internal organs and blood vessels. There are also complex types of sensitivity: two-dimensional-spatial feeling, localization, discriminatory sensitivity, stereognosis, etc.

The English neurologist Ged (N. Head) proposed to divide the general sensitivity into protopathic and epicritical. Protopathic Ch. is phylogenetically older, associated with the thalamus, and serves to perceive nociceptive stimuli that threaten the body with tissue destruction or even death (for example, strong pain stimuli, sudden temperature effects, etc.). Epicritical Ch., phylogenetically younger, is not associated with the perception of damaging effects. It enables the organism to navigate in the environment, to perceive weak stimuli, to which the organism can respond with a choice reaction (an arbitrary motor act). Epicritical Ch. include tactile, low temperature fluctuations (from 27 to 35 °), irritation, their difference (discrimination), and muscular-articular feeling. Decrease or function of epicritical Ch. leads to disinhibition of the function of the protopathic Ch. system and makes the perception of nociceptive irritations unusually strong. At the same time, pain and temperature stimuli are perceived as especially unpleasant, they become more diffuse, spilled and do not lend themselves to precise localization, which is indicated by the term "".

Special Ch. is associated with the function of the sense organs. It includes Vision , Hearing , Smell , Taste , Body balance . Taste Ch. is associated with contact receptors, other types - with distant receptors.

Ch.'s differentiation is connected with structural and physiological features of a peripheral sensitive neuron - its receptor and a dendrite. Normal for 1 cm 2 skin has an average of 100-200 pain, 20-25 tactile, 12-15 cold and 1-2 heat receptors. Peripheral sensory nerve fibers (dendrites of the cells of the spinal node, trigeminal node, jugular node, etc.) conduct excitatory impulses at different speeds depending on the thickness of their myelin layer. Group A fibers, covered with a thick layer of myelin, conduct an impulse at a speed of 12-120 m/s; group B fibers, which have a thin myelin layer, drive impulses at a speed of 3-14 m/s; group C fibers - unmyelinated (have only one) - at a speed of 1-2 m/s. Group A fibers serve to conduct impulses of tactile and deep Ch., but they can also conduct pain stimuli. Group B fibers conduct pain and tactile stimuli. Group C fibers are conductors of mainly pain stimuli.

The bodies of the first neurons of all types of Ch. are located in the spinal ganglia ( rice. one ) and in the nodes of sensory cranial nerves (Cranial nerves) . The axons of these neurons, as part of the posterior roots of the spinal nerves and the sensory roots of the corresponding cranial nerves, also enter the brain stem, forming two groups of fibers. Short fibers end in a synapse at the cells of the posterior horn of the spinal cord (their analogue in the brainstem is the descending spinal tract of the trigeminal nerve), which is the second sensitive neuron. The axons of most of these neurons, having risen by 2-3 segments, pass through the anterior white commissure to the opposite side of the spinal cord and go up as part of the lateral spinothalamic tract, ending in a synapse at cells of specific ventrolateral nuclei of the thalamus. These fibers carry pain and temperature pulses. Another part of the fibers of the spinothalamic pathway, passing through the simplest types of tactile sensitivity (, hair sensitivity, etc.), is located in the anterior funiculus of the spinal cord and makes up the anterior spinothalamic tract, which also reaches the thalamus. cells of the nuclei of the thalamus (third sensitive neurons) axons, forming the posterior third of the posterior thigh of the inner capsule, reach the sensitive neurons of the cerebral cortex (cerebral cortex) ( posterior central and parietal).

A group of long fibers from the posterior root passes uninterruptedly in the same direction, forming thin and wedge-shaped bundles. As part of these bundles, axons, without crossing, rise to the medulla oblongata, where they end in nuclei of the same name - in the thin and wedge-shaped nuclei. Thin (Goll) contains fibers that conduct Ch. from the lower half of the body, wedge-shaped (Burdaha) - from the upper half of the body. The axons of the cells of the thin and sphenoid nuclei pass at the level of the medulla oblongata to the opposite side - the upper sensitive medial loops. After this decussation in the suture, the fibers of the medial loop go up in the posterior part (tire) of the pons and midbrain and, together with the fibers of the spinothalamic tract, approach the ventrolateral nucleus of the thalamus. Fibers from the thin nucleus approach the cells located laterally, and from the sphenoid nucleus - to more medial groups of cells. The axons of sensitive cells of the nuclei of the trigeminal nerve also fit here. neurons of the thalamic nuclei, axons pass through the posterior third of the posterior thigh of the internal capsule and, ending at the cells of the cortex of the postcentral gyrus (fields 1, 2, 3), the upper parietal lobule (fields 5 and 7) of the cerebral hemispheres. These long fibers carry out muscular-articular, vibrational, complex types of tactile, two-dimensional, spatial, discriminatory Ch., feelings of pressure, stereognosis - from the receptors of the same half of the body to the medulla oblongata. Above the medulla oblongata, they reconnect with conductors of pain and temperature sensitivity of the corresponding side of the body.

Research methods sensitivity is divided into subjective and objective. Subjective methods are based on the psychophysiological study of sensation (absolute and differential thresholds of sensitivity). Clinical study Ch. (see Examination of the patient , neurological examination) should be carried out in a warm and quiet room. In order to better focus on the perception and analysis of sensations, he should lie with his eyes closed. The results of Ch.'s research depend on the patient's reaction, his attention, the safety of consciousness, etc.

Pain sensitivity is examined by a pin prick or other sharp object; temperature - by touching the skin with test tubes filled with cool (not higher than 25 °) and hot (40-50 °) water. More accurately, temperature Ch. can be examined using a thermoesthesiometer, and pain - with a Rudzit algesimeter. The threshold characteristic of pain and tactile sensitivity can be obtained by examining graduated bristles and hairs using the Frey method. Tactile Ch. is examined by lightly touching the skin with a brush, pieces of cotton wool, soft paper, etc. Discriminatory Ch. is examined with Weber's compass. Normally, two separate irritations on the palmar surface of the fingers are perceived when one is removed from the other by 2 mm, on the palmar surface of the hand, this distance reaches 6-10 mm, on the forearm and dorsum of the foot - 40 mm, and on the back and hips - 65-67 mm.

The muscular-articular feeling is examined in the position of the patient lying down, always with his eyes closed. produces an unsharp passive in individual small or large joints - extension, adduction, etc. The subject must determine the direction, volume and these movements. You can use a kinesthesiometer. With a pronounced violation of the muscular-articular feeling, a sensitive (Ataxia) .

The feeling of pressure is determined by distinguishing pressure from a light touch, and also by detecting the difference in the degree of pressure applied. The study is performed using a baresthesiometer - a spring apparatus with a pressure intensity scale expressed in grams. Normally, it distinguishes between an increase or decrease in pressure on the arm by 1/10 - 1/20 of the original pressure.

Vibrating frequency is examined with a tuning fork 64-128 Hz. The leg of a sounding tuning fork is placed on protrusions (ankles, forearms, iliac crest, etc.). Normal vibration at the ankles lasts 8-10 from, on the forearm - 11-12 from.

The ability to recognize two-dimensional stimuli is examined by asking the patient to determine, with his eyes closed, the numbers, letters and figures that he draws with a pencil or the blunt end of a pin on the skin of the subject.

The stereognostic sense is defined by the ability to recognize coins, a pencil, a key, etc. when touched with closed eyes. The subject evaluates the shape, consistency, temperature, surfaces, approximate mass and other qualities of the object. The complex act of stereognosis is associated with the associative activity of the brain. With the defeat of general types of sensitivity, this is impossible - secondary (pseudoastereognosis). Primary happens with a disorder of higher brain (cortical) functions - gnosis (see Agnosia) .

Sensitivity disorders are often observed in various diseases of the nervous system and, as a rule, are used to clarify the tonic diagnosis, as well as to control the dynamics of the pathological process under the influence of the patient's treatment. Distinguish between quantitative and qualitative violations of Ch. Quantitative are a decrease in the intensity of sensation - or a complete loss of Ch. -. This applies to all types of Ch., analgesia - a decrease or absence of pain Ch., thermoanesthesia - a decrease or absence of temperature Ch., topohypesthesia, topanesthesia - a decrease or loss of localization of irritation, etc. An increase in Ch. - is associated with a decrease in the threshold of perception of one or another irritation . Qualitative disturbances of Ch. include a perversion of the perception of external stimuli, for example: the occurrence of a sensation of pain during cold or thermal irritation, a sensation of a larger size of a palpated object - macroesthesia, a sensation of many objects instead of one - polyesthesia, a sensation of pain in another zone in relation to the injection site - synalgia, sensation of irritation not in the place of its application - alloesthesia, sensation of irritation in a symmetrical area on the other hand -, inadequate perception of various irritations -. Ch. represents a special form of qualitative change - a kind of painful perception of various sharp irritations. With hyperpathy, excitability increases (light irritations are perceived less clearly in the zone of hyperpathy than normal, and intense irritations are sharply painful, extremely unpleasant, painful), irritations are poorly localized by the patient, they are noted for a long time.

Ch.'s disorders include paresthesia - various sensations not associated with any external influence - goosebumps, numbness, tingling, stiffness of skin areas, pain in the hair roots (trichalgia), a feeling of skin moisture, drops of liquid on it (). Especially often, a variety of paresthesias are observed with dorsal tabes (Tapes dorsalis) , funicular myelosis (Funicular myelosis) and other diseases of the nervous system, in which the posterior cords of the spinal cord and posterior roots are involved in the process.

Depending on the localization of the pathological process in the nervous system, various types of disorders of Ch. are observed. When the receptor apparatus is damaged, a local one is observed due to a decrease in the number of receptor points, as well as changes in the threshold characteristics of different types of Ch. .

When a sensory nerve is damaged, two zones of disturbance are detected: anesthesia in the zone of autonomic innervation of this nerve and hypesthesia with hyperpathy in the zone of mixed innervation (overlapping of innervation zones with another nerve). There is a discrepancy between the zones of violation of various types of Ch.: the largest surface is occupied by the area with violation of the temperature Ch., then the tactile one, and least of all - the area of ​​violation of the painful Ch. relatively high temperature (above 37 °) and low (below 20 °), injections are perceived as extremely unpleasant, diffuse, long-lasting sensations. Later (about 1 year later), tactile sensitivity is restored, the ability to distinguish between temperatures from 26 to 37 °, at the same time, the localization error and increased pain stimuli disappear (Ged's - Sherren's law). With damage to the peripheral nerve, all types of sensitivity are disturbed (see Neuritis) . For multiple symmetrical lesions of the peripheral nerves of the extremities (see Polyneuropathies) characteristic is a violation of all types of Ch. according to the polyneuritic or distal type - in the form of gloves on the hands and stockings (socks) on the legs ( rice. 2 ).

With damage to the posterior roots, disorders of all types of Ch. are localized in the corresponding dermatome ( rice. 3 ). With a viral lesion of the spinal node and sensitive root, paresthesia and hypesthesia are combined with herpetic eruptions in the same dermatome (see Ganglionitis) .

With the defeat of the entire diameter of the spinal cord, a conductor of all types develops with an upper border, which indicates the level of the spinal cord ( rice. 4 ). With the localization of the pathological focus above the cervical thickening of the spinal cord, the upper and lower extremities, the trunk appear. This is combined with central tetraparesis, dysfunction of the pelvic organs (see spinal cord) . The pathological focus at the level of the upper thoracic segments is manifested by anesthesia on the lower extremities, central lower paraparesis, and dysfunction of the pelvic organs. When the lumbar segments of the spinal cord are affected, conduction anesthesia captures the lower limbs and the anogenital zone.

The pathology of the thalamus causes Dejerine-Roussy, in which all types of Ch. decrease or disappear on the half of the body opposite to the focus, sensitive and moderate develop in the same limbs, contralateral hemianopsia . Characteristic of the defeat of the thalamus is hyperpathy and central against the background of hypesthesia on the entire half of the body. Thalamic pain is always very intense, diffuse, burning and resistant to analgesics.

With the defeat of the posterior thigh of the internal capsule, the so-called capsular one develops on the half of the body opposite to the focus. It is characterized by more pronounced Ch.'s disorders in the distal extremities, especially on the arm.

A pathological focus in the radiant crown or cerebral cortex ( postcentral) causes monoanesthesia on the face or only on the arm, or only on the leg (depending on the location of the focus and in accordance with the somatotopic representation of sensitivity). with cortical pathological foci, it is more pronounced in the distal parts of the limb, and the muscular-articular feeling and vibrational frequency are more disturbed than the superficial frequency.

When the pathological process is localized in the parasagittal region, both paracentral lobules are simultaneously disturbed and sensitivity is impaired on both feet.

Irritation of the sensitive zone of the cerebral cortex (with, cicatricial adhesive process, etc.) leads to Jacksonian sensitive seizures (see Jacksonian epilepsy) : paresthesias in the face, arm or leg, lasting from a few seconds to minutes without a change in consciousness. With damage to the parietal lobe, more complex types of Ch.'s disturbance develop, a weakening of the ability to discriminate, two-dimensional-spatial Ch., stereognosis, and to determine spatial relationships (topognosis).

Bibliography: Krol M.B. and Fedorova E.A. The main neuropathological syndromes, M,. 1966; Skoromets A.A. diseases of the nervous system, L., 1989.

Rice. 4. Scheme of conduction spinal paraanesthesia with an upper limit on Th X .

Rice. 1. Scheme of conductors of superficial (A) and deep (B) sensitivity: 1 - cell of the spinal ganglion; 2 - cell of the posterior horn of the spinal cord; 3 - spinothalamic tract; 4 - ; 5 - postcentral gyrus (zone of the leg); 6 - cell of the spinal ganglion; 7 - Gaulle's bundle; 8 - Gaulle's beam core; 9 - bulbotalamic tract ().

II Sensitivity

the ability of the body to perceive irritations emanating from the environment or from its own tissues and organs.

Visceral sensitivity(s. visceralis) - Ch. to irritations acting on internal organs.

Sensitivity of taste(s. gustatoria) - Ch. to chemical action, realized by the appearance of a sensation of taste of the active substance.

Sensitivity deep(s. profunda) - see Proprioceptive sensitivity.

Directional sensitivity- Ch. to some properties of the environment, realized by spatial orientation, the allocation of a certain direction in it.

Sensitivity discrimination(s. discriminativa) - Ch., which consists in the ability to distinguish between two simultaneous identical irritations of different localization, for example, in different areas.

Sensitivity differential(s. differentialis; Ch. difference) - a variety of Ch., which consists in the ability to perceive a change in the intensity of irritation.

Sensitivity interoceptive(s. interoceptiva) - H. to irritations emanating from the internal environment of tissues and organs.

Skin sensitivity(s. cutanea) - Ch. to irritation of various (tactile, temperature, pain) skin receptors.

Nociceptive sensitivity(s. nociceptiva) - see Pain sensitivity.

Olfactory sensitivity(s. olfactoria) - Ch. to chemical effects, realized by the appearance of the smell of the influencing substance.

Surface sensitivity(s. superficialis) - see Sensitivity exteroceptive.

Sensitivity proprioceptive(s. proprioceptiva; synonym: deep sensitivity) - C. to irritation of muscles, tendons, ligaments and other elements of the joints.

Protopathic sensitivity(s. protopathica; Greek prōtos first, primary + pathos feeling, suffering,) is a phylogenetically ancient Ch., characterized by limited possibilities for differentiating stimuli according to their modality, intensity and localization.

Sensitivity difference- see Differential sensitivity.

light sensitivity(s. visualis) - H. to the effects of visible radiation.

Sensitivity is difficult(s. composita) - Ch., based on the integration of the activity of receptors of various modalities.

Hearing sensitivity(s. auditiva) - H. to the effects of sound.

temperature sensitivity(s. thermaesthetica) - Ch. to a change in ambient temperature.

Sensitivity exteroceptive(s. exteroceptiva; syn. Ch. superficial) - Ch. to irritations emanating from the environment.

Electrodermal sensitivity(s. electrocutanea) - a kind of skin Ch., which consists in the ability to perceive when exposed to electric current.

When the words " hypersensitivity», « sensitive person", and even the term that has already become commonplace sounds - HSP (highly sensitive people), it becomes immediately clear that we are talking about something that goes beyond the average, a certain statistical majority.

Many, having heard hypersensitivity" And " sensitive person", imagine a kind of muslin young lady, regardless of her real gender, who just faints" from an excess of feelings.

Someone thinks that all this is whims, and it is enough to “get together”, “stop winding yourself up”, and immediately this sensitivity will pass. All this, they say, from spoiled.

Still others, who are in the minority, believe that hypersensitivity- a gift, sensitive person, most likely talented and prone to creativity.

Let's try to figure out what HSP actually is, and, most importantly, how to live with it among those whose level of sensitivity is mostly lower.

Obviously, if there is an increased sensitivity, there is also a certain average, one might say - a statistical majority, something that many are accustomed to starting from as a norm.

Sensitivity in general is the ability of the human nervous system to perceive various stimuli coming from outside and respond to them. If you do not delve into the structure of the nervous system and physics, then in general we can say that human sensitivity exists within certain limits.

For example, human hearing recognizes sounds in the range of 20 - 20,000 hertz, or light human sensitivity is in the range of 380 - 760 nm, but everything that is inside these frames has very individual shades.

For example, to one person, the conversation of neighbors behind the wall will seem like a light, barely noticeable noise. Others won't hear anything at all. Third, every word will be heard. So it can be with color and with other sensations - taste, smells, touch. So it can be with the sensation of pain - any physician will tell you that the pain threshold in a person is individual.

At the same time, the researchers note that the population of the earth by about 10% - hsf highly sensitive people. The rest have the same average sensitivity, which is usually perceived as the norm. Rarely, there are cases of complete or partial loss of sensitivity, which are mainly associated with diseases of the central nervous system or with severe psychological shocks.

Why is that? Here, scientists so far agree that hypersensitivity is an innate characteristic. It is difficult to say how much it is determined by hereditary factors, because in some cases it is possible to observe the appearance of children with high sensitivity in the family of parents with average indicators.

True, no one will say for sure whether at least one of the child's parents really did not have hypersensitivity, or whether he simply actively suppressed it and skillfully hid it. There is not much research on this topic yet, but so far there are a few obvious signs of HHL.

HSP signs

Physical

This is the very case when the conversation of the neighbors behind the wall seems loud and distinct to you, unlike the others. You are annoyed by pungent odors, too bright light, you are susceptible to light touches, distinguish the slightest shades of taste, temperature, your body reacts quite noticeably to many interventions - drugs, caffeine, other psychoactive and stimulant substances, you have a lower pain threshold (pain comes earlier , from less visible stimuli than for most).

emotional

You have a heightened sense of empathy, you are quite easily imbued with the situation of another person and easily "pick up" his emotions, it is easy for you to feel the state of the people around you, sometimes - regardless of your desire, you easily feel the atmosphere of some place, you are more receptive to art, you are able to experience strong emotions from “little things”.

intellectual

You carefully consider and weigh your words, any incoming information, you tend to reflect on it, you have increased attention to details, nuances (for example, you notice grammatical errors and typos, are susceptible to all sorts of carelessness, negligence in the surrounding space, which others may not notice at all for a long time), you are able to see many meanings in any external object.

This division, of course, is conditional - it is impossible to disassemble a person, like a mechanism, into parts, therefore, of course, everything is connected. But a sensitive person is not necessarily one who has all the senses "at the limit."

For example, he may have very high auditory and visual sensitivity, while he may show a normal pain threshold, or, say, not be highly sensitive to drugs. Or, let's say a person has high empathy, but he is not inclined to go deep into intellectual meanings.

Therefore, now we will talk about what nuances high sensitivity has, touching on common myths about sensitivity, talk about how this is related to other aspects of a person’s psychological characteristics - for example, introversion / extraversion, psychotype, temperament, degree of neuroticism, and maybe whether it be a symptom of some other condition, disease.

In general, hypersensitivity is not an innate characteristic of a person, but a consequence of certain conditions of the body. For example, sensitivity can increase in the presence of chronic lack of sleep, constant fatigue, severe stress (as, however, a partial decrease in sensitivity can also be a reaction to stress, as if “freezing” in the presence of very strong and indigestible feelings).

Hypersensitivity may accompany some mental disorders and somatic diseases, especially those associated with the central nervous system. But this mention is only so that you can decide for yourself whether your characteristic is permanent or temporary. Here we will talk mainly about those whose hypersensitivity is constant, you yourself remember yourself like this all your life, and you have not observed any other serious deviations in the field of health.

So far, I have not come across studies in which it would be clearly possible to trace which psychotypes are more often associated with increased sensitivity. However, our own practice gives enough reason to assert that hypersensitivity is neither the fifth type of temperament, nor any special psychotype, HSPs are found among representatives of different temperaments and psychotypes.

It can be said that some psychotypes appear among HSPs more often than others, but a clear correlation has not yet been traced. That is, a sensitive person can be born like that with any other character traits.

Many people assume that HSPs tend to be more introverted. This is logically understandable: a sensitive person needs more time to recover from contact with the outside world, because external stimuli act on him more than others, and he needs to disconnect from strong stimulation more often.

But I have also met extroverts among HSPs. Yes, such a person also needed to retire from time to time, to have time to recover, but the focus of attention of such a person was still directed to the outside world, and not to the inner one, like introverts.

With temperament, too, it is not possible to establish a clear connection. It would be logical to assume that HSPs are more suited to people with fast arousal and slow deceleration, in other words, they are easy to turn on but difficult to calm down (which are melancholics), but this is more like speculation about what a sensitive person should be, or could be in the opinion of the middle majority, not on reality.

And the logic can be completely different. Sometimes heightened sensitivity fits, say, into a phlegmatic person who does not look like a sensitive person at all. However, the phlegmatic temperament creates good protection for the carrier of subtle sensitivity, and it even blossoms inside him in a lush color, since outwardly she is little threatened.

In general, here we can say that hypersensitivity is not directly related to specific features of the psychotype, temperament or focus of attention, it exists as a separate psychophysiological characteristic that is built into other personality parameters.

But a person not only experiences feelings, he also interprets them. For example, the fact that he is more responsive to the people around him and their condition, needs more rest from this stimulation, he can interpret in different ways.

He can calmly say to himself: “Yes, it’s already too much for me today, I want to be in silence” - and calmly retire. Or he can start to wind himself up in the spirit of “all people are like people, but I’m not like that, probably something is wrong with me, since everything starts to annoy me so quickly ....”

Often, HSPs are confused with people who are prone to anxiety, suspiciousness and thinking out on this basis for others. But heightened sensitivity and anxiety reinforced by fantasies are two different things.

A sensitive person will be able to capture the real state of another person - for example, he will be able to feel that his boss has already entered the office irritated and tense, and further scatter of employees only followed from his initial state. Therefore, a sensitive person is unlikely to take it personally. However, he may be hurt for another reason - too loud, too bright, too hard.

But an anxious person just may not feel the real state of the authorities, he is mainly occupied with his experiences, and therefore he will easily attribute the dressing to himself alone, and then he will worry for days about his alleged worthlessness and failure.

It is also easy to confuse people who are able to loudly and vividly demonstrate their feelings (it is not at all necessary that the feelings are sincere, and that they generally exist) with HSP. But demonstration and real feeling are very different things. HSPs are just not in a hurry to share their feelings so quickly, much less so loudly: the demonstration attracts attention even more, makes them digest a lot more external stimuli, and further exacerbates the fatigue from their own reactions.

And here it is very logical to mention a few common myths about sensitivity.

HSP: myths and reality

In fact, rather the opposite. Among them there are many generally strong people who control their feelings sometimes much better than representatives of the average majority.

Why? Yes, because from childhood, such a child understands that he is different from others, that his feelings are sometimes not taken seriously by others. Parents and other adults are not always ready to take feelings (and even more so - so subtle!) into account and sometimes even declare them abnormal.

Naturally, in response to this, the child develops defenses. And one of them is the formation of the skill of tracking and controlling your emotions. Yes, sometimes this leads to sad options - a habit is formed to suppress their feelings, low self-esteem, a feeling of constant misunderstanding and rejection.

But increased sensitivity also gives its bonus, especially in the presence of high intelligence: after all, the mass of feelings inaccessible to others is a mass of information, this is a more complete and rich knowledge of the world, this is a more subtle insight into the essence of human motives and relationships, and as a result - a more effective strategy of action, and in the long run - a more comfortable place in life.

In general, HSPs are less likely to act rashly “on emotions”, they are more likely to think about the nuances of their reactions and behavior, they can more effectively cope with difficult life situations, if only because life taught them very early to cope with their feelings in world less sensitive.

Myth: A sensitive person is open, kind, and therefore very vulnerable.

This is also from the realm of fantasy. HSPs tend to keep their feelings away from others most of the time, or at least that's what experience teaches them. Not every closed person belongs to the HSP category, but we can say that among the HSPs there are many who are considered to be closed. And, especially having the experience of different perceptions of themselves, HSPs are very selective in communication.

The capacity for empathy, which HSPs certainly have to a large extent, is not a reason for kindness, let alone naivete. The experience of subtle feeling can be applied in many ways, but think about it: subtle sensitivity involves feeling all spectrums.

And this means that a sensitive person feels not only the wonderful feelings of people filled with positive. In principle, there are not enough of them in the world, to put it mildly. And it turns out that the main content of empathy is a very different, and far from always positive state of people.

What conclusions can HSPs draw from this? - yes, whatever. You can find yourself in a helping profession in order to attach this empathy, to give it a place. And you can hate the whole human race for the constant violation of boundaries and for that very joyless inner content. And for example, to become a charming villain like Hannibal Lecter, who, in addition to killing, enjoys delicate dishes from their liver or brain, decorates the house with exquisite paintings and listens to rare performances of the opera.

Therefore, in terms of moral guidelines, HSPs can be at any pole of society, and sensitivity will only impart certain shades to their actions, but it in no way limits their choice in terms of their own ethics.

Myth: Sensitive people are talented and smart

This is partly true, of course, because hypersensitivity itself is an indication for certain types of activities in which it is needed - many areas of art and science (especially where intuition matters), in general, a creative environment that helps professions - psychologists, doctors, social workers.

But at the same time, increased sensitivity also imposes certain restrictions - for example, a sensitive person cannot always work in the conditions in which the majority can work. And sometimes it becomes an obstacle to career development in the standard way accepted in society and a particular profession.

I have known people who have a high sensitivity combined with low intelligence. This is perhaps the most difficult of all HSPs, because they do not have enough resources to realize their uniqueness, while they also do not always succeed in fully integrating into the world of ordinary people.

In summary, HSPs are simply people with a distinct characteristic that goes along with different personality traits. Of course, increased sensitivity to one degree or another leaves an imprint on the formation of a psychotype, and on interaction with temperament, and on behavioral habits.

And this is certainly a variant of the norm, which, however, differs from the majority and creates certain problems for such people. And in the next part of the article, we will dwell in more detail on the development of a sensitive child and talk about what parents should do, whose child is just like that: “ Sensitive child: features of the development of a sensitive person».

Of all the characteristics of speakers and acoustic systems, the concept of “sensitivity” is perhaps the most interesting and attractive (in this it competes with the power characteristic). One would like this concept to be directly dependent on the quality of the speaker, i.e. the larger this parameter, the better the speaker sounds. After all, an acoustic system is a device for playing music, and its quality is often determined only in a subjective way, and sensitivity - from the word feel, feeling well, subconsciously merges with the word quality. However, we know that this is not the case. First of all, this concept is purely technical, reflecting the efficiency of the speaker. According to GOST 16122-78, the characteristic sensitivity of the speaker is the ratio of the average sound pressure developed by the speaker in a given frequency range (usually 100 ... 8000 Hz) on the working axis, reduced to a distance of 1 m and an input electric power of 1 W. Of course, if we have a speaker with a higher sensitivity, then by supplying 1 W we will get more sound pressure than from a speaker with a low sensitivity, less non-linear distortion and, probably, a higher sound quality. However, it is worth considering how this sensitivity is obtained?

We have several legal (real) and illegal (marketing) ways to increase sensitivity.

Real ways to fight for sensitivity

Speaker systems with a large number of speakers

When connecting several speakers (acoustic systems) in parallel (in series), the volume level increases (and the power also increases). It is used for sound systems and due to the variability in the characteristics of broadband speakers, the sound quality remains low. Often the method is used in acoustic systems where 2 or more woofers are used for one tweeter. In this case, the main problem is the features of the directivity characteristic of such a system.

Increasing the sensitivity of single speaker systems

The speaker, acoustic system is an electro-mechanical-acoustic transducer and, as a result, it is possible to increase the efficiency of the system at each stage of this transformation.

Electro-mechanical coupling factor (BL) speaker

The first stage is electro-mechanical transformation. For this, the coefficient "BL" is introduced. It depends on "B" - induction in the gap and "L" - the length of the conductors in this gap (or the number of conductors on which the magnetic field acts). "B" can be increased by increasing the volume and strength of the magnets, reducing the magnetic gap both in height and in width. "L" - increasing the diameter of the coil and the number of turns in height in the gap. If you increase the value of "BL", without changing the other characteristics of the speaker, then the sensitivity in the area above the main resonance of the speaker will increase, and the low-frequency capabilities will remain unchanged.

Mass of the moving system

By reducing the mass of the moving system, we can create more pressure than with a larger mass. This improves the impulse and transient characteristics, but reduces strength (power), stiffness (nonlinear distortion may increase) and will require the use of new materials and technologies. Receiving low frequencies, especially deep ones, requires a lot of effort.

Radiation area

Increasing the area of ​​the diffuser leads to an increase in the level of sensitivity, but there are problems with the reproduction of high frequencies and the strength of the structure.

Acoustic transformation - horn

This method allows you to get low frequencies from a small and light speaker by matching it with the environment. It requires a lot of effort in terms of building buildings. The most competent, but also the most expensive way.

Well-designed loudspeakers with really high sensitivity use the last four methods, and sometimes the first. As shown, this requires spending large amounts of money, increasing the cost of the system and increasing its size, however, you can do it easier.

Illegal way

Recall that the sensitivity is measured on the axis, at a distance of 1 meter when summing up 1 W of power. How to get this 1W? To do this, you need to determine the nominal resistance. It is selected from a range of 2, 4, (6), 8, 16, 25 and 50 ohms. Since the speaker is a complex resistance with a complex dependence of the electrical impedance modulus on frequency, the definition of this resistance obeys the law. For example, this is written in GOST 9010-84 “The measured minimum value of the electrical impedance modulus in the range lying above the fundamental resonance frequency should not differ from the nominal electrical resistance by more than minus 20%.” Thus, the value of the total electrical resistance modulus of a 4-ohm system cannot be less than 3.2 Ohm, and of an 8-ohm system - 6.4 Ohm, etc. Then, according to Ohm's law, to measure a speaker with a nominal resistance of 4 Ohms, we must bring 2 Volts (root of 4) to it, 8 Ohms - 2.82V, and for 16 Ohms - 4 V.

In Western descriptions and passports, the “sensitivity” column is often found, with a characteristic of 1m / 2.8V, in combination with “resistance”, for example, 6 ohms. When measuring, it turns out that the minimum resistance of such a product is 3.4 Ohm. So the system turns out to be really 4 Ohm, and we apply 2 W to it (According to Ohm's law 2.8V2 / 4 \u003d 2W) and we get a sensitivity increase of 3 dB. In addition to this, the frequency response, especially of the speakers separately, has areas of dips and rises, which makes it possible to fix the sensitivity in the area of ​​this rise. Not to mention the possibility of a simple postscript. As a result, we easily get an increase in the sensitivity value of 4-8 dB. The measurement of acoustic systems of Western manufacturers, including eminent ones, unfortunately, showed that this practice is common and is used, with rare exceptions, everywhere.

What is it for?

It's all about low frequencies, because. the level of low frequencies when indicating the frequency range in the passport, and when listening, is measured precisely from the average sound pressure level - sensitivity and, therefore, systems with real low sensitivity have a gain in the number and depth of low frequencies. And getting deep low frequencies and high sensitivity with a certain size of speakers and acoustic systems is very difficult. After all, you can’t write a sensitivity of 80dB in your passport, no one will buy it! It is much easier to write a normal level of sensitivity and when listening to give the client a mighty bass.

This text is not written to accuse someone of falsification, but to provide the consumer with more complete information.